menu

Nudging does not necessarily improve decisions

Listen to this article

Nudging does not necessarily improve decisions

Nudging, the concept of influencing people’s behavior without imposing rules, bans or coercion, is an idea that government officials and marketing specialists alike are keen to harness, and it’s often viewed as a one-size-fits-all solution. Now, a study puts things into perspective: Whether a nudge really does improve decisions depends on a person’s underlying decision-making process.

A well-known example of nudging is placing the salad bar near the cafeteria entrance to promote a healthy diet. It has been shown that this simple change has an effect on the food people choose to eat for lunch. However, is a light salad really the best option from the employee’s perspective, or is it their employer who will benefit from staff who perform better in the afternoon? And,is improving the decisions we make really that simple?

What the researchers say: Whether a nudge ultimately results in a person making decisions that are better suited to their needs is an important factor in assessing the effectiveness of nudges. This is the starting point of this research. How do you measure whether a nudge improves a decision in the eyes of the person being nudged?

“We can’t determine whether a nudge improves the choices a person makes until we understand how they reach their decisions,” says the lead researcher putting the hype surrounding nudging into perspective. “Depending on which behavioral model we take as a starting point, it is possible to measure the effectiveness of nudges—or not?”

Traditional economics assumes that a person’s preferences can be inferred from their decisions and behavior. According to the rational behavior model, a person’s decision to have a salad or a steak for lunch is based on which meal meets their needs. When it comes to assessing nudges, however, this model is problematic, since nudging manipulates precisely the behavior that is supposed to shed light on a person’s preferences. The researchers therefore looked to alternative behavioral models to determine the assumptions under which a nudge can be assessed in a meaningful way.

According to the “satisficing” model, a person will consider their alternatives and subsequently choose the first one that meets their needs in a satisfactory way. The person will order the salad because it is the first option that adequately fulfills their requirements. Although they might have enjoyed the steak more, they will not consider that option, since they have already made up their mind. In this model, hardly any conclusions can be drawn about the true preferences of a person, and their decisions cannot be improved through nudging either.

If we assume decisions are made according to the limited attention model, however, the situation changes: This model is based on the idea that a person will only ever consider a certain number of possibilities—for example, only the first three meals on a menu that features five options. The person will then ponder these options and choose the best meal out of this selection. Unlike with the satisficing model, conclusions can be drawn about a person’s preferences. Decisions that are based on such a decision-making process can be improved by nudging. Therefore, if you know that a salad is indeed an ideal meal, then placing it among the first three items on the menu will ensure that a person will at least consider this meal and maybe also choose it.

It is therefore necessary to know what a person’s true needs and preferences are in order to assess the success of nudges when it comes to improving decisions. If we don’t have this information, any nudging that takes place is done without knowing what is in a person’s best interests. “Our findings show that the success of nudging greatly depends on how we view the human decision-making process,” says the lead researcher. “We can’t conclusively determine whether nudging makes sense as long as current scientific knowledge in economics, psychology and neuroscience doesn’t allow nudging to be assessed in a consistent manner.”

So,what? Besides the fact that nudging may not actually work for most people there are also ethical problems attached to it? Is it right to try and influence people in ways that they neither understand or are aware of? Further you might argue that nudging for a “good” end result is OK (the end justifies the means?) but wrong for a “bad” outcome. The problem here is who decides what is “good” and what is “bad”?

On balance I believe that nudging is unethical. Now we find that for many it may also be useless

Dr Bob Murray

Bob Murray, MBA, PhD (Clinical Psychology), is an internationally recognised expert in strategy, leadership, influencing, human motivation and behavioural change.

Join the discussion

More from this issue of TR

No items found.

Join our tribe

Subscribe to Dr. Bob Murray’s Today’s Research, a free weekly roundup of the latest research in a wide range of scientific disciplines. Explore leadership, strategy, culture, business and social trends, and executive health.

Thank you for subscribing.
Oops! Something went wrong while submitting the form. Check your details and try again.