menu

Using robotic pets in memory care

July 31, 2022

Listen to this article

Using robotic pets in memory care

You might think it was a typical therapy session at a long-term care facility. In a quiet room, a therapist sets down a pet carrier, brings out a cat, and sets it on a resident’s lap. As the resident gently strokes the cat’s fur, it purrs, and the therapist asks the resident questions about their childhood pets, accessing long-ago memories.

The resident’s enjoyment of the session and the benefit for their well-being is real. But the animal is not. It’s a robotic pet with synthetic fur and programmed movements and sounds. Researchers are finding that robotic pets can be useful in therapy, without some of the disadvantages and unpredictability of real animals.

In a paper published in the Canadian Journal of Recreation Therapy researchers developed a protocol for using robotic pets with older adults with dementia. The protocol uses a low-cost robotic pet, establishes ideal session lengths, and identifies common participant responses to the pets. This will aid in future research.

What the researchers say: “Our protocol had questions like: Would you like to scratch the dog behind his ears? Would you like to pet him? Would you like to brush him?” said the lead researcher. “We evaluated how people responded to those different cues so that we could then provide some guidelines on how to have the most beneficial actions with these animals.”

The lead researcher watched the development of robotic pets for the past decade, intrigued by the potential to use them therapeutically in long-term and geriatric care settings. But until recently the price was prohibitive. “Having been a therapist myself and training our students to work as therapists, I’m very aware that most facilities would never be able to purchase them.”

But with the introduction of robotic pets priced at under $150, widespread use of them as therapy “animals” seemed within reach. Robotic pets can get around many of the risks and drawbacks of live animals in long-term care settings. Many facilities don’t allow personal pets because of allergies, the potential for bites or scratches, and other reasons.

Researchers had already begun to study how people with dementia interact with robotic pets but hadn’t yet developed a unified protocol to give, say, assisted living staff a way to get the most benefit from the pets’ use through directed interaction.

“There was very little information on what people were doing with the pets,” the lead author said. “So without that guidance, it’s just a toy. And what do you do with it?”

Before the COVID-19 pandemic, the researchers met with people between 82 and 87 years old living in long-term care facilities who experienced severe cognitive impairment. In two sessions of 30 minutes each, the researchers brought out either a robotic dog or a cat (participants’ choice) in a pet carrier.

“Many participants leaned toward the [robotic pet] as it was taken out of the pet carrier,” the researchers observed, “then instinctively reached for it and began petting, rubbing or scratching the pet when first introduced.”

Throughout the session, the researchers asked questions, both about the participants’ experiences with past pets and about interacting with the current robotic pet. “Did they have dogs or cats?” “What were their names? Did they keep them indoors or outdoors? What types of food did they eat?” were typical questions.

The researchers carefully observed the responses of the participants to the pets. The robotic pets moved and made sounds, which helped the participants engage with them.

“When the dog would bark they would say things like, ‘Oh, are you trying to tell me something?’” the researchers said. “Or they would comment on the cat purring and would say things like, ‘Wow, you must be happy! I feel you purring.’ One of the activities that people responded to the most was brushing the animals.”

In one case, though, the session proceeded in silence. The participant had difficulty communicating their thoughts but stayed focused on the robotic dog throughout. By the end of the session, the participant seemed to develop a connection with the robotic animal, saying “I like that dog. When he likes me.”

The researchers were often asked if the participants with cognitive decline understand that the robotic pets are not alive. In this study, they said, they all seemed aware that it was not a live animal.

“We would never tell somebody that it was live if they asked. We would be honest with them. We usually introduce it as ‘Would you like to hold my dog’ and people react to it or respond to it in a way that’s meaningful for them.”

All of the participants enjoyed the activity, with several saying they liked it “very much.” One participant didn’t like the sounds the pet made, which was easily remedied by turning off the sound—not an option for a live animal.

The questions that spurred the most response related to personal reminiscences and directions for interacting with the pet.

A common, yet unprompted behavior, the researcher’s report, was communication with the pet. “Several participants used comments, sounds, specific inflections, and facial expressions spontaneously with the pets,” the researchers wrote. “Some participants imitated the animal sounds made by the [pet] and repositioned the pet to look at its face or make eye contact.”

Although more research is needed to determine the optimal session length, the researchers noted that the 30-minute sessions in the study were sufficient. They hope to explore how people with varying levels of cognitive decline respond to the pets, as well as how they can be used in a group setting.

The study found that the most meaningful interactions and the most enjoyable experiences came when the participant self-directed the session.

“In recreational therapy, we always talk about providing person-centered care,” the researchers said. So it’s not really about what I think about an activity. If somebody enjoys it and it brings happiness to them, then it’s really about what they think about it.”

Why does interacting with robotic pets provide such an enjoyable experience?

“People in long-term care facilities are in a position where everybody provides care to them,” the lead author said, “and to be in the role where you are nurturing something else, or you are the caregiver I think is also psychologically very comforting for people to feel like, even though they know that it’s not live, they’re the person who’s giving love and compassion to something, and it’s responding.”

So, what? Animal therapy has a long and successful history. When Alicia and I had a psychotherapy practice in New York, our dog Biscuit and our cat Penny proved very useful adjuncts. They were calm and responsive and added a great deal to the effectiveness of the sessions. They were particularly useful in cases of depression and anxiety.

We’ve noticed that our present dog, Maud, has a similar effect. Like Biscuit she is calm with people and responsive to their moods.

I think it’s unfortunate that more of the sorts of institutions that the present researchers visited don’t allow real dogs in these sessions. I believe a trained therapy dog would achieve a much better and more lasting outcome than a robot.

Dr Bob Murray

Bob Murray, MBA, PhD (Clinical Psychology), is an internationally recognised expert in strategy, leadership, influencing, human motivation and behavioural change.

Join the discussion

Join our tribe

Subscribe to Dr. Bob Murray’s Today’s Research, a free weekly roundup of the latest research in a wide range of scientific disciplines. Explore leadership, strategy, culture, business and social trends, and executive health.

Thank you for subscribing.
Oops! Something went wrong while submitting the form. Check your details and try again.